The twelve theories of co-existence in plant communities: the doubtful, the important and the unexplored
- J. Bastow Wilson
Article first published online: 29 NOV 2010
DOI: 10.1111/j.1654-1103.2010.01226.x
© 2010 International Association for Vegetation Science
Keywords:
- Aggregation;
- Chance;
- Circular interference networks;
- Co-evolution;
- Competitive ability;
- Cyclic succession;
- Disturbance;
- Environmental fluctuation;
- Initial patch composition;
- Interference/dispersal trade-offs;
- Niche differentiation;
- Paradox of the Plankton;
- Pest pressure;
- Spatial mass effect
Abstract
Background: Twelve distinct explanations have been proposed for the co-existence of species in ecological communities.
Types of mechanism:
The mechanisms can be divided into those that are stabilizing, i.e.
with an increase-when-rare mechanism, and those that are equalizing, the
latter on their own only delaying the exclusion of species. However, by
evening out fitness, equalizing mechanisms can facilitate the operation
of stabilizing mechanisms.
Importance:
It is suggested that circular interference networks, co-evolution of
similar interference ability, cyclic succession, equal chance
(neutrality) and initial patch composition are likely to be unimportant,
or perhaps not even occur. Equal chance is an equalizing mechanism.
Allogenic disturbance, alpha-niche differentiation, environmental
fluctuation (relative non-linearity and/or the storage effect) and pest
pressure are probably important. All four are stabilizing. More evidence
is needed on aggregation, interference/dispersal trade-offs and the
spatial mass effect. Aggregation and the spatial mass effect are
equalizing. Suggestions are made of the evidence needed to make informed
judgements on which contribute the most to co-existence in plant
communities.
Introduction
Ecology
has few laws, but the one almost invariable feature of plant
communities is that they contain more than one species. Long-term
studies such as the Park Grass experiment (
Silvertown 1987) show us that much of this is persistence rather than transience. Indefinite co-existence of species contravenes the logic of
Gause (1934)
that the species with the highest interference ability, i.e. the
highest long-term relative population growth rate (PGR) in admixture
with other species, should oust all others.
Hutchinson (1941, 1961)
termed this the “Paradox of the Plankton”, asking: “How [is it]
possible for a number of species to co-exist in a relatively isotrophic
or unstructured environment, all competing for the same sorts of
materials?” The same question arises for all vegetation. Having combed
the literature and sorted synonyms, I find there are 12 distinct
mechanisms proposed and possible (
Wilson 1990). The 12 are compared with other classifications of co-existence mechanisms in
Table 1. Those classifications have been hierarchical, but the differences are actually multi-faceted (
Table 2).
Table 1.
Various classifications of mechanisms of species
co-existence. The original mechanism names that can best be recovered
from the original paper are given, with the names used here in italics for comparability.
Connell (1978) |
Non-equilibrium hypotheses |
Intermediate disturbance hypothesis: allogenic disturbance |
Equal chance |
Gradual change: environmental fluctuation1 |
Equilibrium hypotheses |
Niche diversification: alpha-niche differentiation |
Circular networks: circular interference networks |
Compensatory mortality |
Herbivores attack and kill seeds or seedlings of common species more frequently: pest pressure |
Cyclic succession |
Apparently not considered |
Co-evolution of similar interference ability |
Initial patch composition |
Interference/dispersal trade-offs |
Spatial mass effect |
Aggregation |
Wilson (1990) |
Density-dependent mechanisms |
Niche diversification: alpha-niche differentiation |
Pest pressure |
Competitive exclusion is a weak force |
Equal chance |
There is never time for competitive exclusion to operate |
Gradual climate change: environmental fluctuation2 |
Intermediate-timescale disturbance: allogenic disturbance |
Colonization mechanisms |
Life-history differences: Interference/dispersal trade-off |
Initial patch composition |
Spatial mass effect |
Continuous autogenic change |
Circular competitive networks: circular interference networks |
Cyclic succession |
Aggregation (including temporal inertia) |
Stabilizing co-evolution: co-evolution of similar interference ability |
Bengtsson et al. (1994) |
Large-scale processes |
Immigration–extinction dynamics (metapopulation dynamics, mass effect) |
Close similarity in competitive ability: equal chance |
Trade-off between dispersal and competitive ability: Interference/dispersal trade-offs |
Mass effect: spatial mass effect |
Centrifugal organisation of plant communities: [beta-niche differentiation] |
Processes within the community |
Based on niche differences |
Niche differentiation: alpha-niche differentiation |
Gradual change: environmental fluctuation1 |
Regeneration niche: allogenic disturbance |
Based on similarities in competitive ability |
Environmental filtering: equal chance |
Competitive combining ability: co-evolution of similar interference ability |
Lottery models: equal chance / environmental fluctuation1 |
Species aggregation: aggregation |
Apparently not considered |
Circular interference networks |
Cyclic succession |
Initial patch composition |
Pest pressure |
Chesson (2000) |
Stable co-existence |
Fluctuation-independent mechanisms |
Resource partitioning: alpha-niche differentiation |
Frequency-dependent predation: pest pressure |
Fluctuation-dependent mechanisms (relative non-linearity and storage effect): environmental fluctuation |
Spatial effects |
Spatial storage effect and spatial relative non-linearity: spatial mass effect |
Competition–colonization trade-offs: allogenic disturbance and interference/dispersal trade-offs |
Unstable co-existence: equal chance |
Not seen as a mechanism of co-existence: aggregation |
Apparently not considered 3 |
Circular interference networks |
Co-evolution of similar interference ability |
Cyclic succession |
Initial patch composition |
|
Barot & Gignoux (2004) |
Stabilizing processes |
Space+time as resources: cyclic succession |
Space as a resource |
Exogenous heterogeneity |
Regional co-existence: [beta-niche differentiation] |
Disturbance: allogenic disturbance |
Endogenous heterogeneity |
Deterministic recruitment limitation |
Janzen-Connell: pest pressure |
Colonization–competition trade-off: Interference/dispersal trade-offs |
Heteromyopia: Initial patch composition4 |
Limited resource transport: equal chance+aggregation+spatial mass effect |
Time as a resource |
Exogenous variability |
Disturbance: allogenic disturbance |
Each species is the best competitor in given local environmental conditions that are available during given periods: environmental fluctuation1 |
Endogenous variability [no mechanism indicated] |
Equalizing processes |
Small fitness differences, trade-off: equal chance |
Intraspecific aggregation: aggregation |
Demographic stochasticity: equal chance |
Recruitment limitation as a rule: equal chance+aggregation (spatial and temporal) +pest pressure+Interference/dispersal trade-offs |
Ecological drift: equal chance |
Apparently not considered |
Circular interference networks |
Co-evolution of similar interference ability |
Table 2. A classification of mechanisms of species co-existence.
| | | Patch model | Uniform community model |
|
Stabilizing | Requiring allogenic changes | Requiring only competitive interactions | Allogenic disturbance3Interference/dispersal trade-offs3 | Environmental fluctuation |
| Requiring non-competitive interactions | Initial patch composition | – |
Not requiring allogenic changes | Requiring only competitive interactions | Cyclic succession | Alpha-niche differentiation Co-evolution of similar interference ability |
| Requiring non-competitive interactions | – | Circular interference networks1Pest pressure2 |
Equalizing | | | Aggregation | Equal chance Spatial mass effect4 |
There is a basic distinction between stabilizing mechanisms and equalizing mechanisms (
Chesson 2000).
For indefinite co-existence, some stabilizing mechanism must operate,
i.e. one that includes an increase-when-rare process. Moreover, species
abundances always fluctuate and an increase-when-rare mechanism is
required to counter this. Equalizing processes, which simply decrease
the differences between species in replacement rates (i.e. fitness), do
not contain an increase-when-rare mechanism, but by slowing replacement
may allow a weak stabilizing mechanism to overcome a fitness difference (
Chesson 2000).
It is also possible for a stabilizing mechanism to operate as an
equalizing one: even if its stabilizing effect is not strong enough
alone to permit long-term co-existence, (increase-when-rare), it can
reduce the fitness difference between species sufficiently that another
weak stabilizing mechanism can effect co-existence.
Terminology notes:
- 1
“Interference” is used here when “competition” has sometimes been used
in the past, in order to include non-competition forms of interference,
such as allelopathy and red/far-red effects, especially since we rarely
know for sure what type of interference is involved. “Competition” is
used
sensuClements et al. (1929), Grime (2001) and
Begon et al. (2006).
- 2
“PGR” is used to include the whole of population growth – number of
individuals, plant size and vegetative reproduction – thus generalising r, which is commonly used with the number of “individuals”. RGR could well be generalised in the same way.
- 3
“Increase-when-rare” is used to mean locally rare, sparse perhaps, with no implication of global rarity.
The problem of spatial scale and the environment
The spatial scale (grain) of the question is impossible to define.
Warming (1909)
described documenting the fact that different species grow in different
places and hence under different environmental conditions (i.e. in
different beta niches) as an “easy task”. There is considerable current
interest in such “environmental filtering”, particularly in documenting
the extent to which fine-scale vegetation mosaics are caused by
environmental heterogeneity. However, I am not concerned with these
here, however small the scale. Several of the mechanisms do involve
spatial patchiness (
aggregation, allogenic disturbance, cyclic
succession, initial patch composition, interference/dispersal
trade-offs, spatial mass effect), but by definition of the paradox
none rely on allogenic environmental heterogeneity within the area where
co-existence is being questioned.
The
focus is the paradox of how species co-exist locally, in one community,
in one small patch of two-dimensional space. But what is “locally”? The
appropriate spatial scale (grain) is the one at which we look at the
community and ask: “how can the species be co-existing?” but this is
impossible to define and it will differ greatly between communities.
Shmida & Wilson (1985) regarded 10
2–10
4 m
2 as the upper limit of the scale. A quadrat of 10
2 m
2 is commonly used for forests, and 0.5
2–2
2 m
2
in grassland, but sometimes habitat heterogeneity and therefore
expression of beta-niche differences are clear on a scale smaller than
this, e.g. bryophyte species responding to microhabitat differences on
logs. The real scale at which we need to ask questions about
co-existence is the one at which beta-niche explanations fail and a
paradox exists.
Aim
The 12 mechanisms of co-existence have been known for decades, at the latest since the 1920s (
equal chance), 1940s (
alpha-niche differentiation, cyclic succession and
environmental fluctuation), 1950s (
interference/dispersal trade-offs), 1960s (
circular interference networks, co-evolution of similar interference ability and
pest pressure), 1970s (
allogenic disturbance, initial patch composition and
spatial mass effect) and 1980s (
aggregation). By 1990 it was possible to review these 12 mechanisms (
Wilson 1990,
q.v. for history of the mechanisms) and to my knowledge and careful
watching no distinct mechanisms have been described since. In 1990 I
exhaustively evaluated the evidence on these mechanisms for one region,
and evidence has been gradually accumulating worldwide since then. This
paper gives an overview and evaluation of progress so far.
My
particular aim is to consider which mechanisms are likely to be
important, so they are organised into three sections: doubtful
(unimportant or unrealistic), important and unexplored (more
investigation is needed). The truth is that all need more evidence and I
indicate what evidence we need for each. Estimates of the contribution
of the 12 mechanisms to real communities are frankly
guesswork/speculation, and are labelled as such.
The doubtful: unimportant or unrealistic mechanisms
Circular interference networks – stabilizing
Interference
relations between a set of species are said to be non-transitive if the
species cannot be arranged in a pecking order, i.e. an order in which a
species higher in the order is always able to displace one lower down.
Non-transitivity implies that there is at least one circular
interference network (
Fig. 1).
Laird & Schamp (2006)
modelled communities with different networks, and more than one species
persisted only in those containing a circular network. A rare species
will increase rapidly because the species that it can displace will be
abundant.
Circular networks have never been found for plants. For example,
Roxburgh & Wilson (2000a) found a perfect pecking order between seven lawn species.
Taylor & Aarssen (1990)
claimed evidence for circularity, but in fact their three species (and
it is circularity between species that concerns us here) show a perfect
competitive hierarchy of
Phleum pratense→
Agropyron repens→
Poa pratensis.
They claim non-transitivity between genotypes of different species. Out
of 435 pair-wise competitive interactions they found 94 significant at
P<0.05
without protection for multiple significance tests. There are three
genotype–genotype interactions that are at variance with complete
transitivity. Since with 435 possible interactions we would expect 21
spurious Type I significances it seems likely that the three are some of
these 21.
Lack of evidence for
non-transitivity is reasonable, since it is hard to think of a mechanism
for it. There cannot be a network of the type shown in
Fig. 1
that is based solely on competition for the same limiting factor. It is
possible that the species might themselves change the resource that is
limiting. For example, species A may be water-spending and cause water
stress when it is competing with C or B, yet when C and B are competing
without A water may be abundant and competition may be for another
factor. The details of the mechanism seem unlikely, and there has been
no analysis of whether this would work: by algebra, simulation or
experiment. So far as we know, for there to be circular networks,
interference of some other type must be involved, such as allelopathy.
Speculation: I suggest that circular interference networks are extremely rare, or non-existent.
Evidence required:
Clearly, interference abilities of a number of species are needed in a
field-realistic situation. In a mixture with no other stabilizing
mechanism, the species that will take over is that with the higher
population growth rate (PGR, via seeds or vegetative growth) in the
mixture (
Connolly 1997).
Since the relative interference abilities may vary with the proportions
of the two species, the relevant PGRs will be those near equilibrium.
Practical problems are that: (a) waiting for near-equilibrium and then
measuring PGRs will be lengthy; (b) with several species, the experiment
becomes large, replication has to be low, and usually most effects are
non-significant; and (c) if a third (or more) species can co-exist by
some other mechanism, two-species interference abilities may not be
relevant.
Co-evolution of similar interference ability – stabilizing
Aarssen (1983)
suggested that, in a mixture of two species, stronger selection
pressure on the one with lower interference ability would cause it to
become the stronger in interference of the two. Superiority in
interference would therefore alternate between the two species'
populations, an increase-when-rare situation.
Recent
work has emphasised that, as agronomists have long known, genotypes of a
species can differ widely in their interference ability (
Whitlock et al. 2007).
We know that a population, put into a different environment, will
usually change genetically, and if that change is by selective screening
it can occur on a time-scale of a few decades, even months (
Walley et al. 1974).
Martin & Harding (1981)
found evidence of genetic adaptation in interference ability, although
circumstantial because the comparison was of field populations.
Aarssen (1989) presented evidence that
Senecio vulgaris, grown with
Phleum pratense for three generations, increased in its interference with the latter. The
S. vulgaris
study is a rare example of this being shown for a biotic environment.
Presumably, the increase in interference ability in this experiment
could not continue indefinitely.
Speculation:
The mechanism in its “alternating superiority” form would be
stabilizing, but this form seems unrealistic because: (a) even if there
is selection in the subordinate species it is hard to see how it could
overshoot the other species, and (b) it is hard to see how the two
species could increase their interference abilities indefinitely.
Without overshoot, this would be a process working towards the
equalizing equal chance mechanism (see below). It would be
operating on an evolutionary time-scale, although recent work is tending
to suggest this can sometimes be quite short.
Evidence required: An experiment like that with
Senecio vulgaris/
Phleum pratense would be needed, but should be continued for longer and with selection possible in both species.
Becks et al. (2010)
demonstrated by modelling and by experiments with plankton that
co-evolution (here defence against herbivory) can occur surprisingly
quickly. Similar work with vascular plant–plant co-evolution would be
fascinating.
Cyclic succession – stabilizing
The increase-when-rare mechanism here is similar to that of
circular interference networks,
but the cycle is between vegetation phases, not individual species.
Moreover, it can operate with only two phases. Patches are involved, but
their environmental differences are autogenic, not allogenic.
Watt (1947)
excellently reviewed the subject, but unfortunately none of his
examples have been supported by subsequent work. The evidence of
Yeaton (1978)
from a shrub and a cactus in an American desert is interesting,
although no actual succession was observed; a hypothesis was made from
spatial comparisons with evidence for suggested mechanisms. There seems
to be no unequivocal documentation of
cyclic succession.
Speculation: In the light of inability to document cyclic succession, there must be doubt as to whether it exists.
Evidence required:
Permanent quadrats could, over a possibly long time, give evidence if
one knew in advance where to put them. Long-term records, such as those
of Park Grass, Rothamsted, UK, and The Desert Laboratory, Tucson, USA,
might just give examples. Ideally, there would be evidence/experiments
to indicate the mechanism of species replacement at each stage.
Equal chance (neutrality) – equalizing
It is a long-standing idea that there is an element of chance in which species occurs at a particular spot (
Lippmaa 1939; Sale 1977).
Chance will make a much larger contribution to species composition when
the interference abilities of the species and of individual plants are
close to equal (
Hubbell 2001; Chesson & Rees 2007).
Speculation:
equal chance is the equalizing mechanism
par excellence. Although
Connell (1978)
emphasised the chance aspect, the core of this mechanism is the
similarity in interference ability, i.e. the neutrality. Practically,
all species must be morphologically different and this requires them to
be physiologically and functionally different, so the
equal chance mechanism can never be the sole mechanism of co-existence (
Chave 2004).
Equal chance
is simply a statement that whereas between species with different
interference ability one will exclude the other, that process will occur
more slowly when the difference is less. The equality in interference
ability could arise from co-evolution (see above), but will more
commonly be caused by ecological sorting, i.e. the screening out of
species with low interference ability.
Evidence required:
Munday (2004)
demonstrated that two fish species seemed to have very similar
ecological distributions and equal interference abilities, although with
priority effects;
Siepielski et al. (2010) found a similar situation with two damselfly species. It would be interesting to find a similar situation with plants.
Equal chance
is a component (but only one) of Hubbell's “Unified Neutral Theory”,
and almost all tests of the latter have been weak: failure to find
departure from null-model predictions (e.g.
Lieberman & Lieberman 2007). The same is true for
equal chance
co-existence: proving equality is impossible, but confidence limits
around very small differences in interference ability would be
compelling.
Initial patch composition – stabilizing
Levin (1974)
proposed a model in which two species occupy small, transient patches.
Some patches will by chance have more individuals of one species than
the other. If between-species interference is greater than
within-species interference, the species in the majority will suppress
the other in that patch. Although the model involves spatial
differences, the patches are identical in environment so this is not
beta-niche differentiation.
Speculation:
The condition of greater between-species than within-species
interference is beloved of ecological modellers, but seems unlikely for
plants in the real world. It cannot happen via competition, because
resource use will always be at least as similar within-species as
between species (I am considering co-existence of species here, not
genotypes). Mutual allo-allelopathy might provide a mechanism, but it is
very difficult to demonstrate that allelopathy is significant in the
field. More likely mechanisms can be envisaged for animals, such as
mutual predation and behavioural interference. “Heteromyopia” (
Barot & Gignoux 2004) is the same mechanism but without explicit explanation of how the patches arise.
Evidence required:
(a) the existence of the small-scale meta-community structure described
above; (b) that within-species interference is less than
between-species interference; and (c) that this type of interference is
ecologically significant. It seems unlikely all three will occur and can
be demonstrated in the same situation.
Important mechanisms
Alpha-niche differentiation – stabilizing
Gause's
principle says that no two species can permanently occupy the same
niche, but if species differ in their alpha niche (resource niche:
Pickett & Bazzaz 1978; Wilson 1999),
exclusion by interference could be prevented. The increase-when-rare
process is that when a species is rare the resource that it particularly
takes up and requires will be present in greater abundance, although
luxury uptake of nutrients would complicate this process (cf.
Revilla & Weissing 2008). The niche differentiation can include nutrient source (e.g. NO
3- versus NH
4+), nutrient requirement (
Titman 1976;
notably Si for diatoms), depth of rooting, phenology of growth,
flowering and fruiting (the latter two via reduced competition for
pollinators/dispersers), light requirement, etc. Niches can also be
constructed by other species, a type of benefaction, notably for
epiphytes and parasites, or via heterotrophs, notably N
2 fixation by
Rhizobium spp. The “Forest architecture” and “Stratification” mechanisms (
Kohyama & Takada 2009) are clearly
alpha-niche differentiation,
but personally I would like to see an ecological explanation of the
increase-when-rare aspect, and closer matching of the models with
observed data.
Speculation: Niche
differences are visible in all communities, although sometimes it is
difficult to see all the species as having different niches. We rarely
know whether the niche differences are sufficient for co-existence. It
is not easy to think of a single experiment where niche differentiation
has been shown to lead to stability. However, since all species have by
definition different niches, as an act of blind faith, I declare that it
must often be important.
Evidence required:
There have been attempts to conclude, from failure to find niche
differences between co-existing species, that co-existence is not due to
alpha-niche differentiation (e.g.
Mahdi et al. 1989).
Clark et al. (2007) point out that negative evidence is unreliable because niche differences can be due to an interaction of niche axes.
Silvertown (2004)
made the point that there is evidence for various types of niche
differentiation in plant communities, but full documentation of the
alpha niches for all of the species present in a community would be
needed in order to determine for which species co-existence might be due
to
alpha-niche differentiation (complete information is sometimes available for the phenology niche of trees).
Experiments
on mesocosms with different numbers of species can give evidence that
there is over-yielding and greater reliability in mixtures of species,
although the results have been controversial, and rarely is it known
that this is due to niche differentiation, nor what the differences
might be. For example,
Tilman et al. (2006)
examined plots at Cedar Creek sown and maintained to a range of species
richness. Greater reliability (lower variation in biomass from year to
year) was seen in plots with more species. We can suspect that with more
species more niches were occupied, and we can see reliability as an
informal guide to stability. However, evidence to demonstrate that the
mechanism could have been niche differentiation would be very helpful.
Often mixtures that include legumes and non-legumes over-yield most,
presumably due to niche differentiation. For a full demonstration, a
test for true stability sensu
May (1973) is needed (cf.
Roxburgh & Wilson 2000a, b).
In
all the stabilizing mechanisms the question arises of how strong the
effect has to be in order to ensure co-existence, given the particular
differences in fitness between the species and the operation of any
other mechanisms, but the question is asked especially for
alpha-niche differentiation because of the limiting-similarity concept and calculations of
MacArthur & Levins (1967).
This remains unanswered for the real world. There are three pieces of
information available: (a) a theoretical limiting similarity can be
calculated (
Szilágyi & Meszéna 2009); (b) the existence of limiting similarity can be seen in the field, but not the exact limiting-similarity value (e.g.
Stubbs & Wilson 2004);
and (c) the stabilization effect can be measured from the field or
experimentally but without our knowing how much of this is due to
alpha-niche differentiation (
Adler et al. 2007).
These three need to be brought together, perhaps with experimental
removal of the opportunity for niche differentiation or of other
mechanisms.
Environmental fluctuation – stabilizing
Environmental
fluctuation – seasonal, annual and decadal change – does not
necessarily give co-existence. In the absence of any explicitly
stabilizing process, one out of co-occurring species must have the
greatest long-term PGR over such fluctuations, and will in the absence
of other mechanisms of co-existence eliminate its associates.
Chesson (2008) has shown that either of two mechanisms can result in stable co-existence via
environmental fluctuation:
relative non-linearity and the
storage effect, and only these two can do so.
Relative non-linearity
requires that two species differ in the shape of their response to a
resource. All plants affect their environment (“reaction”:
Clements 1904),
and the two species with different response shapes will have different
reactions. The effect of environmental fluctuation on the balance
between the two species and their different reactions will necessarily
give an increase-when-rare mechanism, but which of the two species has
the higher PGR depends on the degree of environmental fluctuation.
The storage effect
is that a rare species will be able to take advantage of environmental
conditions that are favourable for it because there will be little
competition. Four conditions are required: (1) the species are competing
for a resource; (2) they respond differently to an environmental (i.e.
non-resource) factor; (3) competition impacts more on a species in more
“favourable” conditions, i.e. when the species is present with higher
biomass,; and (4) the effect of competition on the population of a
species depends on the environment, and this implies some persistence of
the population through environmental fluctuations, i.e. buffering. The
term “regeneration niche” is difficult to pin down precisely, but seems
to be part of item (2) above.
Speculation: The four conditions for the
storage effect seem almost inevitable, so it must be present often. The conditions for
relative non-linearity are also obvious, but
Chesson's (2008) calculations show it to be a much weaker force. I therefore conclude that
environmental fluctuation will often cause stabilization, primarily through the
storage effect.
Evidence required:
Chesson's models are very significant for theories of co-existence.
Their parameters need to be determined for real-world co-existing
organisms, and found to be consistent with the models. The closest
approaches so far have been by
Adler et al. (2006) in a Kansas prairie,
Verhulst et al. (2008) with shrubs in a Mexican desert,
Angert et al. (2009) with annuals in an Arizona desert, and
Adler et al. (2009) in Idaho sagebrush. These last workers found the processes required for the
storage effect were very weak, but of course models that are correct and realistic will not operate in all communities.
Pest pressure– stabilizing
All
types of pest – herbivores, from insects to large mammals, and also
pathogens – have the potential to give an increase-when-rare process.
For this, three conditions are required.
- 1
Impact: the pests involved must significantly reduce the fitness of the
plant species in terms of survival, growth and/or reproduction.
- 2
Abundance-dependence: the challenge from pests must be less on a species
when it is rare than when it is abundant. The requirement is for a
lower impact on the fitness of a species when rare, but this will usually be through reduced grazing/infection.
- 3
Host specificity: the plant species must differ in the extent to which their fitness is reduced by particular pests.
If
these three conditions are all met, then when any one of the plant
species in the mixture becomes more abundant, its host-specific pest
(Condition 3) will move more rapidly among its host population and the degree of infestation/infection will increase (Condition 2). This will reduce its fitness (Condition 1), but will not directly impact other species, or will do so only to a lesser extent (Condition 3).
Conversely, when a species becomes rare and in danger of being
eliminated, infestation by its specific pests will decrease and its
fitness increase: increase-when-rare.
Any
environmental condition that slows growth will tend to slow species
replacement, but also slow stabilizing mechanisms of co-existence.
Non-selective shoot herbivory may reduce competition for light.
Competition for light is asymmetric because it is based on height
difference, and therefore cumulative, with the results of competition
changing competitive abilities (
Wilson 1988).
Non-selective herbivory will reduce this height-based process and thus
may reduce species replacement more than it slows stabilizing
mechanisms, representing an equalizing mechanism. This is also a
reminder that the 12 known mechanisms cannot be seen in isolation.
Speculation: Seeds and seedlings are often heavily predated (Condition
1) and abundance-dependence (
2) has often been shown (e.g.
Jonzen et al. 2002). Specificity (
3)
is the problem. There is a huge literature assuming that bird
granivores are restricted by beak size to a particular range of
fruit/seed sizes, but not to one species. Most mammal and insect
granivores are not specific to one species, although species of Bruchid
beetle are generally restricted to the seeds of one or a few species of
Fabaceae. Seedlings are eaten by invertebrates, but they are likely to
be quite generalist in their munching.
Invertebrate and vertebrate herbivores of vegetative parts cause appreciable damage (Condition
1, e.g.
Carson & Root 2000), but abundance-dependence (
2) is less clear. Again, host specificity (
3)
is the main issue. With a few notable exceptions, insect vegetative
herbivores are not host-specific, even though they have preferences.
Vertebrates of vegetative parts have facultative preferences but they
are usually polyphagous, some necessarily so since their bite size is
larger than individual plants. They often live longer than their
herbaceous food plants and are mobile, so in response to reduced plant
availability they will switch food or hunt out preferred species rather
than decline in population size, having the reverse effect from that
required. Pathogens often have a significant impact (Condition
1, e.g.
Mihail et al. 1998) and there is evidence for abundance-dependence (
2, e.g.
Mitchell et al. 2002). The weak link is how often pathogens are species-specific (
3), and this seems to vary between ecosystems, from rarely to usually (
Gilbert 2002; Mitchell et al. 2002).
In
view of the above, I do not believe that herbivory/granivory is
generally an effective stabilizing mechanism. Pathogens will often be a
major force for co-existence via the pest pressure mechanism.
Evidence required: Information on impact, specificity and abundance-dependence are required simultaneously for single systems.
Yamazaki et al. (2009)
have made a start by recording seedling mortality and its causes
(disease or herbivory) in relation to seedling density, distance from
conspecific adults and the microenvironment. The evidence could be in
the spatial context of the Janzen-Connell model (Yamazaki et al.'s was,
but the model can operate in equilibrium without the
distance-from-parent element).
The
storage effect mechanism of
environmental fluctuation operates via competition.
Kuang & Chesson (2010) have shown mathematically that since
pest pressure tends to reduce competition, it will reduce the effectiveness of the
storage effect
and hinder co-existence. Data to parameterise these models would be
very useful, and empirical demonstration of the effect would be
fascinating.
Allogenic disturbance – stabilizing
Disturbance is a pulse perturbation that removes biomass from, or kills, plants of most species (
Grime 2001). As a mechanism of co-existence,
allogenic disturbance is a between-patch mechanism (
Wilson 1994),
that is to say, within a local area there are patches that have very
recently been disturbed, others with pioneer species still on them,
others in mid succession and yet others in a climax state. However,
these patches are not different in the allogenic environment.
The
necessary conditions are: (1) there is disturbance of patches at a
scale smaller than the one we are considering for the paradox; (2) this
disturbance occurs with a frequency such that there will a mixture of
patches of different time since disturbance (this is the “Intermediate
Disturbance Hypothesis” of
Connell 1978;
“successional niche” has been used; this may be another aspect of the
rather general term “regeneration niche”); and (3) there are species
that occur with differential frequency in patches of different ages
since disturbance. The process is stabilizing because when a species
inhabiting a particular patch age becomes rare, there is less
within-species interference during its phase of patch succession.
Speculation:
It seems that there are pioneer species (early successional ruderals)
in most communities, but not all. When both pioneer and later
successional species are present, the possibility of the mechanism is
obvious. This was demonstrated in experimental mesocosms by
Questad & Foster (2008).
The question is its importance – when we look at a community not
disturbed in detail by man how much of the heterogeneity that we see is
due to time since disturbance? We do not know, but probably more than we
think (
Wells et al. 2001).
King (1977)
found that only 5% of the plant species in British grasslands were
significantly more abundant on recognizable ant hills, but it is
possible that other species occurred on old ant-hill sites.
Woods (2000)
found that in forests of Michigan, USA, some quadrats (0.8 or 0.1 ha)
contained more of two species he considered successional, and suggested
these were old gaps. Whether these comprise co-existence by
allogenic disturbance depends on the scale examined. I suspect
allogenic disturbance is often an important factor.
Evidence required:
We now know that disturbance is endemic to communities, and can presume
that most communities comprise a mosaic of patches of different times
since disturbance, but this is poorly documented. The pattern of spatial
vegetation heterogeneity needs to be recorded over time, either by
observing changes in permanent quadrats or by tree ageing (since the
disturbance cycle will occur over many decades in forests). It then
needs to be documented that different species occur in the various
post-disturbance successional stages. Most importantly, the latter two
pieces of evidence need to be available for a single community.
The unexplored: mechanisms on which more investigation is needed
Interference/dispersal trade-off – stabilizing
This concept originated with
Skellam (1951) and
Hutchinson (1951). It has been known under a variety of names, including “competition/colonization” and “musical chairs” (
Wilson 1990). The concept is of two species in a habitat that contains transient disturbance patches, intrinsically identical. Species
I is a poor disperser but has strong interference ability, and will take over in all the patches that it reaches. Species
D is a good disperser so it reaches all patches, but it has low interference ability so it will remain only in the patches that
I does not reach. Species
I will eventually reach and dominate the latter patches, but by then disturbance has opened other patches for
D
to colonise. It seems that even the difference in dispersal ability is
not required, just that the dispersal patterns are different (
Berkley et al. 2010).
Speculation: There have been many mathematical models of the mechanism (e.g.
Levins & Culver 1971; Tilman 1994). There is indeed often a negative correlation between interference ability and dispersal ability (
Ehrlén & van Groenendael 1998), and
Turnbull et al. (1999)
gave evidence that large-seeded species, perhaps with lower dispersal
ability, were more dominant when more seeds were sown. However, there is
very little evidence for this mechanism. The scale and timing of
disturbance needs to be right and the trade-off needs to be present,
which make it unlikely to operate often.
Evidence required:
The negative interference/dispersal needs to be documented for the
florule of a community with patchy disturbance, with evidence that the
better dispersers reach all (or most) patches, but the species with
higher interference ability take over patches that they do reach.
Demonstrating this for one community would be an achievement, although
it would not indicate how widespread this situation is.
Spatial mass effect – equalizing?
The
spatial mass effect
occurs when the local population of a species has a negative growth
rate according to its own survival and reproduction (i.e. PGR<0,
λ<1), but is maintained by constant immigration from outside the
community (
Clements 1905). It has also been called the “sink effect”.
Snyder & Chesson (2004) applied and extended the temporal
storage effect and
relative non-linearity (see “Environmental variation” above) to an environmental mosaic, incorporating the
spatial mass effect but also considering persistence of species in the whole landscape, source and sink patches.
Sears & Chesson (2007) parameterised this model.
The
spatial mass effect
is difficult to categorise. It is equalizing in that there is no
increase-when-rare process, yet stabilizing in that there can be
indefinite co-existence. It is a patch model in that it requires an
external patch with a different environment (beta-niche differentiation
on a larger scale) to provide the source of propagules. However, it is a
mechanism of co-existence without patches at the scale examined, and
can certainly explain why we see species co-existing and the mixture
persisting within an intrinsically uniform area (
Table 2).
Evidence
is sparse. A dramatic example occurs in the Lost World Cavern, northern
New Zealand, where 13 species of angiosperm grow without any of them
ever setting seed (
de Lange & Stockley 1987). In a population of
Cakile edentula on a sand dune system,
Keddy (1982) observed that plants at the seaward edge produced many seeds and had high survival (it
is
a strand-line species). However, in the rear dunes, the few plants
surviving to maturity did not produce enough seeds to balance mortality,
and the population was maintained by seeds blown inland from the
strand.
Speculation: Wherever there is vegetational heterogeneity, seeds will surely germinate in foreign territory and ramets invade, so the spatial mass effect will be present almost always.
Evidence required:
The population growth rate within the community must be shown to be
PGR<0, and the propagule influx must be quantified. Both of these are
very difficult to do, and they need to be done for the same community.
Aggregation (Spatial Inertia) – equalizing
Spatial
aggregation of the plants of a species gives inertia, delaying
exclusion by interference since replacement occurs only at patch
boundaries if dispersal is limiting. We assume the patches are
intrinsically identical, in environment but perhaps different in species
for historical reasons. The process was described analytically by
Clifford & Sudbury (1973) and modelled by
Silvertown et al. (1992). It has been demonstrated with real plants in an artificial community (
Stoll & Prati 2001). However, theory shows that aggregation does not necessarily slow down exclusion by interference (
Chesson & Neuhauser 2002) and
Chesson (2008) argues that the effect of aggregation is an artefact caused by not considering the right time-scale.
Temporal Inertia (
Cowles 1901)
refers to the ability of plants to persist for some time when
conditions are unsuitable for them, as adult plants or as a reproducing
but gradually declining population. It is really not a mechanism of
co-existence, simply an artefact of using a time-scale inappropriate for
the life history of the plants.
Speculation: Species are aggregated almost everywhere, so this seems likely to have very wide importance as an equalizing mechanism.
Evidence required: Experiments like that of Stoll and Prati (2001) are needed, but under realistic field conditions.
Conclusion
Since
we see multi-species communities all around us, in apparent breach of
the law of Gause, it is not surprising that 12 explanations have been
proposed. Some are related (
Table 2).
For example, three depend on disturbance. One of these and a fourth
include autogenic succession. The mathematics of the storage effect can
be used to describe the
spatial mass effect, storage in space
rather than time. Five explanations require patches within the area we
are considering, although not in the intrinsic environment, as I have
eschewed such beta-niche effects. However, I believe all 12 mechanisms
are distinct. The need is now for hard evidence from nature on these
mechanisms. Too often, in vegetation science, observation and theory
proceed happily without either bothering the other. That has largely
been the case here.
Stable co-existence
raises the concept of “stability”. Early workers had taken low
variability, now more precisely called “reliability”, as equivalent to
stability. This fails because low variability can also be due to low
perturbation, and anyway how low does the variability have to be in
order for us to call it stability? An “equilibrium” can be stable, or
unstable. The concepts of “resistance” and “resilience”, applied to a
stable community, describe features of the stabilizing process, not the
existence of it, and resistance can be asked also of an unstable
community. Stability
sensu stricto (Lyapunov stability, local
stability) is defined as the ability of a community to return eventually
to its former state after an extremely small pulse perturbation (
May 1973).
Taken from mathematics, it is clear and consistent, and the only one we
can use. Objections are raised that: (a) we cannot observe extremely
small perturbations, (b) we cannot wait for “eventually”, and (c) before
we could wait, another disturbance would happen and the environment
would change, as it is always doing. But we must remember that although
determining stability
sensu stricto is beyond the ability of mortals, observing stabilizing processes is not. The study of
Harpole & Suding (2007)
is an example. It is therefore preferable, and practical, to think of
stabilizing processes, especially the ability to increase when rare.
The
opposite question is why monospecific stands are occasionally seen, in
obedience to Gause's law but as a surprise because of their rarity. They
usually occur in aquatic or intermittently wet habitats such as lakes
and the lower zones of salt marshes, but occasionally in stress habitats
such as saline deserts. I do not know of any theory directed at this.
P.W. Richards suggested that in tropical rain forests, normally diverse,
mono-dominance was to be found in unfavourable habitats (
Salisbury 1931), but
Connell (1979)
suggested that mono-dominance is the climax situation, and the iconic
species-rich tropical rain forest communities are recovering from
disturbance.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements.
I thank for ideas on this topic Peter Adler, Andrew Agnew, Peter
Chesson, Jason Fridley, Robin Pakeman, Mike Palmer, Robert Peet and my
research group.
References
- Aarssen, L.W. 1983. Ecological combining ability and competitive combining ability in plants: towards a general evolutionary theory of co-existence in systems of competition. American Naturalist 122: 707–731.
- Aarssen, L.W. 1989. Competitive ability and species co-existence: a ‘plant's-eye’ view. Oikos 56: 386–401.
- Adler, P.B., HilleRisLambers, J., Kyriakidis, P.C., Guan, Q. & Levine, J.M. 2006. Climate variability has a stabilizing effect on the co-existence of prairie grasses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 103: 12793–12798.
- Adler, P.B., HilleRisLambers, J. & Levine, J.M. 2007. A niche for neutrality. Ecology Letters 10: 95–104.
- Adler, P.B., HilleRisLambers, J. & Levine, J.M. 2009. Weak effect of climate variability on co-existence in a sagebrush steppe community. Ecology 90: 3303–3312.
- Angert, A.L., Huxman, T.E., Chesson, P. & Venable, D.L. 2009. Functional tradeoffs determine species co-existence via the storage effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 11641–11645.
- Barot, S. & Gignoux, J. 2004. Mechanisms promoting plant co-existence: can all the proposed processes be reconciled? Oikos 106: 185–192.
- Becks, L., Ellner, S.P., Jones, L.E. & Hairston, N.G. 2010. Reduction of adaptive genetic diversity radically alters eco-evolutionary community dynamics. Ecology Letters 13: 989–997.
- Begon, M., Harper, J.L. & Townsend, C.R. 2006. Ecology: individuals, populations and communities. 4 ed. Blackwell, Oxford, UK.
- Bengtsson, J., Fagerström, T. & Rydin, H. 1994. Competition and co-existence in plant communities. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 9: 246–250.
- Berkley, H.A., Kendall, B.E., Mitarai, S. & Siegel, D.A. 2010. Turbulent dispersal promotes species co-existence. Ecology Letters 13: 360–371.
- Carson, W.P. & Root, R.B. 2000. Herbivory and plant species co-existence: community regulation by an outbreaking phytophagous insect. Ecological Monographs 70: 73–99.
- Chave, J. 2004. Neutral theory and community ecology. Ecology Letters 7: 241–253.
- Chesson, P. 2000. Mechanisms of maintenance of species diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 31: 343–366.
- Chesson, P. 2008. Quantifying and testing species co-existence mechanisms. In: Valladares, F., Camacho, A., Elosegui, A., Estrada, M., Gracia, C., Senar, J.C. & Gili, J.M. (eds.) Unity in diversity: reflections on ecology after the legacy of Ramon Margalef. pp. 119–164. Fundación Banco Bilbao Vizcaya Argentaria, Madrid, ES.
- Chesson, P. & Neuhauser, C. 2002. Intraspecific aggregation and species co-existence. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17: 210–211.
- Chesson, P. & Rees, M. 2007. Commentary on Clark et al. 2007. Resolving the biodiversity paradox. Ecology Letters 10: 659–661.
- Clark, J.S., Dietze, M., Chakraborty, S., Agarwal, P.K., Ibanez, I., LaDeau, S. & Wolosin, M. 2007. Resolving the biodiversity paradox. Ecology Letters 10: 647–659.
- Clements, F.E. 1904. Studies on the vegetation of the state. III. The development and structure of vegetation. Reports of the Botanical Survey of Nebraska 7: 1–175.
- Clements, F.E. 1905. Research methods in ecology. University Publishing Co., Lincoln, NE, US.
- Clements, F.E., Weaver, J.E. & Hanson, H.C. 1929. Plant competition: an analysis of community functions. Carnegie Institute of Washington, Washington, DC, US.
- Clifford, P. & Sudbury, A. 1973. A model for spatial conflict. Biometrika 60: 581–588.
- Connell, J.H. 1978. Diversity in tropical rain forests and coral reefs. Science 199: 1302–1310.
- Connell, J.H. 1979. Tropical rain forests and coral reefs as open non-equilibrium systems. In: Anderson, R.M., Turner, B.D. & Taylor, L.R. (eds.) Population dynamics. pp. 141–163. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, UK.
- Connolly, J. 1997. Substitutive experiments and the evidence for competitive hierarchies in plant communities. Oikos 80: 179–182.
- Cowles, H.C. 1901. The physiographic ecology of Chicago and vicinity; a study of the origin, development, and classification of plant societies. Botanical Gazette 31: 73–108, 145–182.
- de Lange, P.J. & Stockley, J.E. 1987. The flora of the lost world Cavern, Mangapu Caves System, Waitomo, Te Kuiti. Bulletin of the Wellington Botanical Society 43: 3–6.
- Ehrlén, J. & van Groenendael, J.M. 1998. The trade-off between dispersability and longevity – an important aspect of plant species diversity. Applied Vegetation Science 1: 29–36.
- Gause, G.F. 1934. The struggle for existence. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, MD, US.
- Gilbert, G.S. 2002. Evolutionary ecology of plant diseases in natural ecosystems. Annual Review of Phytopathology 40: 13–43.
- Grime, J.P. 2001. Plant strategies, vegetation processes, and ecosystem properties. 2 ed. Wiley, Chichester, UK.
- Harpole, W.S. & Suding, K.N. 2007. Frequency-dependence stabilizes competitive interactions among four annual plants. Ecology Letters 10: 1164–1169.
- Hubbell, S.P. 2001. The unified neutral theory of biodiversity and biogeography. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, US.
- Hutchinson, G.E. 1941. Ecological aspects of succession in natural populations. American Naturalist 75: 406–418.
- Hutchinson, G.E. 1951. Copepodology for the ornithologist. Ecology 32: 571–577.
- Hutchinson, G.E. 1961. The paradox of the plankton. American Naturalist 95: 137–145.
- Jonzen, N., Nolet, B.A., Santamaria, L. & Svensson, M.G.E. 2002. Seasonal herbivory and mortality compensation in a swan–pondweed system. Ecological Modelling 147: 209–219.
- Keddy, P.A. 1982. Population ecology on an environmental gradient: Cakile edentula on a sand dune. Oecologia 52: 348–355.
- King, T.J. 1977. The plant ecology of ant-hills in calcareous grasslands: I. Patterns of species in relation to ant-hills in southern England. Journal of Ecology 65: 235–256.
- Kohyama, T. & Takada, T. 2009. The stratification theory for plant co-existence promoted by one-sided competition. Journal of Ecology 97: 463–471.
- Kuang, J.J. & Chesson, P. 2010. Interacting co-existence mechanisms in annual plant communities: frequency-dependent predation and the storage effect. Theoretical Population Biology 77: 56–70.
- Laird, R.A. & Schamp, B.S. 2006. Competitive intransitivity promotes species co-existence. American Naturalist 168: 182–193.
- Levin, S.A. 1974. Dispersion and population interactions. American Naturalist 108: 207–228.
- Levins, R. & Culver, D. 1971. Regional co-existence of species and competition between rare species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 68: 1246–1248.
- Lieberman, M. & Lieberman, D. 2007. Nearest-neighbor tree species combinations in tropical forest: the role of chance, and some consequences of high diversity. Oikos 116: 377–386.
- Lippmaa, T. 1939. The unistratal concept of plant communities (the unions). American Midland Naturalist 21: 111–145.
- MacArthur, R. & Levins, R. 1967. The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. American Naturalist 101: 377–385.
- Mahdi, A., Law, R. & Willis, A.J. 1989. Large niche overlaps among coexisting plant species in a limestone grassland community. Journal of Ecology 77: 386–400.
- Martin, M.M. & Harding, J. 1981. Evidence for the evolution of competition between two species of annual plants. Evolution 35: 975–987.
- May, R.M. 1973. Stability and complexity in model ecosystems. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, US.
- Mihail, J.D., Alexander, H.M. & Taylor, S.J. 1998. Interactions between root-infecting fungi and plant density in an annual legume, Kummerowia stipulacea. Journal of Ecology 86: 739–748.
- Mitchell, C.E., Tilman, D. & Groth, J.V. 2002. Effects of grassland plant species diversity, abundance, and composition on foliar fungal disease. Ecology 83: 1713–1726.
- Munday, P.L. 2004. Competitive co-existence of coral-dwelling fishes: the lottery hypothesis revisited. Ecology 85: 623–628.
- Pickett, S.T.A & Bazzaz, F.A. 1978. Organization of an assemblage of early successional species on a soil moisture gradient. Ecology 59: 1248–1255.
- Questad, E.J. & Foster, B.L. 2008. Co-existence through spatio-temporal heterogeneity and species sorting in grassland plant communities. Ecology Letters 11: 717–726.
- Revilla, T. & Weissing, F.J. 2008. Nonequilibrium co-existence in a competition model with nutrient storage. Ecology 89: 865–877.
- Roxburgh, S.H. & Wilson, J.B. 2000a. Stability and co-existence in a lawn community: mathematical prediction of stability using a community matrix with parameters derived from competition experiments. Oikos 88: 395–408.
- Roxburgh, S.H. & Wilson, J.B. 2000b. Stability and co-existence in a lawn community: experimental assessment of the stability of the actual community. Oikos 88: 409–423.
- Sale, P.F. 1977. Maintenance of high diversity in coral reef fish communities. American Naturalist 111: 337–359.
- Salisbury, E.J. 1931. Annual meeting, January 1931. Journal of Ecology 19: 221–222, 224–226.
- Sears, A.L.W. & Chesson, P. 2007. New methods for quantifying the spatial storage effect: an illustration with desert annuals. Ecology 88: 2240–2247.
- Shmida, A. & Wilson, M.V. 1985. Biological determinants of species diversity. Journal of Biogeography 12: 1–20.
- Siepielski, A.M., Hung, K.L., Bein, E.E.B. & McPeek, M.A. 2010. Experimental evidence for neutral community dynamics governing an insect assemblage. Ecology 91: 847–857.
- Silvertown, J. 1987. Ecological stability: a test case. American Naturalist 130: 807–810.
- Silvertown, J. 2004. Plant co-existence and the niche. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19: 605–611.
- Silvertown, J., Holtier, S., Johnson, J. & Dale, P. 1992. Cellular automaton models of interspecific competition for space: the effect of pattern on process. Journal of Ecology 80: 527–534.
- Skellam, J.G. 1951. Random dispersal in theoretical populations. Biometrika 38: 196–218.
- Snyder, R.E. & Chesson, P. 2004. How the spatial scales of dispersal, competition, and environmental heterogeneity interact to affect co-existence. American Naturalist 164: 633–650.
- Stoll, P. & Prati, D. 2001. Intraspecific aggregation alters competitive interactions in experimental plant communities. Ecology 82: 319–327.
- Stubbs, W.J. & Wilson, J.B. 2004. Evidence for limiting similarity in a sand dune community. Journal of Ecology 92: 557–567.
- Szilágyi, A. & Meszéna, G. 2009. Limiting similarity and niche theory for structured populations. Journal of Theoretical Biology 258: 27–37.
- Taylor, D.R. & Aarssen, L.W. 1990. Complex competitive relationships among genotypes of three perennial grasses: implications for species co-existence. American Naturalist 136: 305–327.
- Tilman, D. 1994. Competition and biodiversity in spatially structured habitats. Ecology 75: 2–16.
- Tilman, D., Reich, P.B. & Knops, J.M.H. 2006. Biodiversity and ecosystem stability in a decade-long grassland experiment. Nature 441: 629–632.
- Titman (Tilman), D. 1976. Ecological competition between algae: experimental confirmation of resource-based competition theory. Science 192: 463–465.
- Turnbull, L.A., Rees, M. & Crawley, M.J. 1999. Seed mass and the competition/colonization trade-off: a sowing experiment. Journal of Ecology 87: 899–912.
- Verhulst, J., Montaña, C., Mandujano, M.C. & Franco, M. 2008. Demographic mechanisms in the co-existence of two closely related perennials in a fluctuating environment. Oecologia 156: 95–105.
- Walley, K.A., Khan, M.S.I. & Bradshaw, A.D. 1974. The potential for evolution of heavy-metal tolerance in plants. I. Copper and zinc tolerance in Agrostis tenuis. Heredity 32: 309–319.
- Warming, E. 1909. Oecology of plants: an introduction to the study of plant-communities. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
- Watt, A.S. 1947. Pattern and process in the plant community. Journal of Ecology 35: 1–22.
- Wells, A., Duncan, R.P. & Stewart, G.H. 2001. Forest dynamics in Westland, New Zealand: the importance of large, infrequent earthquake-induced disturbance. Journal of Ecology 89: 1006–1018.
- Whitlock, R., Grime, J.P., Booth, R. & Burke, T. 2007. The role of genotypic diversity in determining grassland community structure under constant environmental conditions. Journal of Ecology 95: 895–907.
- Wilson, J.B. 1988. The effect of initial advantage on the course of competition. Oikos 51: 19–24.
- Wilson, J.B. 1990. Mechanisms of species co-existence: twelve explanations for Hutchinson's ‘Paradox of the Plankton’: evidence from New Zealand plant communities. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 13: 17–42.
- Wilson, J.B. 1994. The ‘Intermediate disturbance hypothesis’ of species co-existence is based on patch dynamics. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 18: 176–181.
- Wilson, J.B. 1999. Guilds, functional types and ecological groups. Oikos 86: 507–522.
- Woods, K.D. 2000. Dynamics in late-successional hemlock-hardwood forests over three decades. Ecology 81: 110–126.
- Yamazaki, M., Iwamoto, S. & Seiwa, K. 2009. Distance-
and density-dependent seedling mortality caused by several diseases in
eight tree species co-occurring in a temperate forest. Plant Ecology 201: 181–196.
- Yeaton, R.I. 1978. A cyclical relationship between Larrea tridentata and Opuntia leptocaulis in the Northern Chihuahuan Desert. Journal of Ecology 66: 651–656.
Nenhum comentário:
Postar um comentário